Reflex- this is the body’s response to irritation from the external or internal environment, carried out with the help of the central nervous system. There are unconditioned and conditioned reflexes.

Unconditioned reflexes- these are congenital, permanent, hereditarily transmitted reactions characteristic of representatives of a given type of organism. For example, pupillary, knee, Achilles and other reflexes. Unconditioned reflexes ensure the interaction of the organism with the external environment, its adaptation to environmental conditions and create conditions for the integrity of the organism. Unconditioned reflexes arise immediately after the action of a stimulus, since they are carried out along ready-made, inherited reflex arcs, which are always constant. Complex unconditioned reflexes are called instincts.
Unconditioned reflexes include sucking and motor reflexes, which are already characteristic of an 18-week fetus. Unconditioned reflexes are the basis for the development of conditioned reflexes in animals and humans. In children, with age, they turn into synthetic complexes of reflexes, which increases the body’s adaptability to the external environment.

Conditioned reflexes- reactions are adaptive, temporary and strictly individual. They are inherent only in one or several representatives of the species, subjected to training (training) or exposure to the natural environment. Conditioned reflexes are developed gradually, in the presence of a certain environment, and are a function of the normal, mature cortex of the cerebral hemispheres and lower parts of the brain. In this regard, conditioned reflexes are related to unconditioned ones, since they are a response of the same material substrate - nervous tissue.

If the conditions for the development of reflexes are constant from generation to generation, then the reflexes can become hereditary, that is, they can turn into unconditioned. An example of such a reflex is the opening of the beak of blind and fledgling chicks in response to the shaking of the nest by a bird flying in to feed them. Since shaking the nest is followed by feeding, which was repeated in all generations, the conditioned reflex becomes unconditioned. However, all conditioned reflexes are adaptive reactions to a new external environment. They disappear when the cerebral cortex is removed. Higher mammals and humans with damage to the cortex become deeply disabled and die in the absence of the necessary care.

Numerous experiments conducted by I.P. Pavlov showed that the basis for the development of conditioned reflexes is formed by impulses arriving along afferent fibers from extero- or interoreceptors. For their formation, the following conditions are necessary: ​​1) the action of an indifferent (future conditioned) stimulus must precede the action of an unconditioned stimulus. With a different sequence, the reflex is not developed or is very weak and quickly fades away; 2) for a certain time, the action of the conditioned stimulus must be combined with the action of the unconditioned stimulus, that is, the conditioned stimulus is reinforced by the unconditioned. This combination of stimuli should be repeated several times. In addition, a prerequisite for the development of a conditioned reflex is the normal function of the cerebral cortex, the absence of painful processes in the body and extraneous stimuli.
Otherwise, in addition to the reinforced reflex being developed, an indicative or reflex of the internal organs (intestines, bladder, etc.) will also occur.


An active conditioned stimulus always causes a weak focus of excitation in the corresponding area of ​​the cerebral cortex. The unconditioned stimulus that is connected (after 1-5 s) creates a second, stronger focus of excitation in the corresponding subcortical nuclei and the area of ​​the cerebral cortex, which distracts the impulses of the first (conditioned) weaker stimulus. As a result, a temporary connection is established between both foci of excitation of the cerebral cortex. With each repetition (i.e. reinforcement), this connection becomes stronger. The conditioned stimulus turns into a conditioned reflex signal. To develop a conditioned reflex, a conditioned stimulus of sufficient strength and high excitability of the cells of the cerebral cortex are necessary, which must be free from external stimuli. Compliance with the above conditions accelerates the development of a conditioned reflex.

Depending on the method of development, conditioned reflexes are divided into secretory, motor, vascular, reflexes of changes in internal organs etc.

A reflex developed by reinforcing a conditioned stimulus with an unconditioned one is called a first-order conditioned reflex. Based on it, you can develop a new reflex. For example, by combining a light signal with feeding, a dog has developed a strong conditioned salivation reflex. If you give a bell (sound stimulus) before the light signal, then after several repetitions of this combination the dog begins to salivate in response to the sound signal. This will be a second-order reflex, or secondary, reinforced not by an unconditioned stimulus, but by a first-order conditioned reflex. When developing conditioned reflexes of higher orders, it is necessary that a new indifferent stimulus is turned on 10-15 s before the onset of the conditioned stimulus of a previously developed reflex. If the stimulus acts at intervals that are closer or combined, then a new reflex will not appear, and the previously developed one will fade away, since inhibition will develop in the cerebral cortex. Repeated repetition of jointly acting stimuli or a significant overlap of the time of action of one stimulus on another causes the appearance of a reflex to a complex stimulus.

A certain period of time can also become a conditioned stimulus for developing a reflex. People have a temporary reflex to feel hungry during the hours when they usually eat. Intervals can be quite short. In children school age reflex for time - weakening of attention before the end of the lesson (1-1.5 minutes before the bell). This is the result not only of fatigue, but also of the rhythmic functioning of the brain during training sessions. The reaction to time in the body is the rhythm of many periodically changing processes, for example, breathing, cardiac activity, awakening from sleep or hibernation, molting of animals, etc. Its occurrence is based on the rhythmic sending of impulses from the corresponding organs to the brain and back to the effector organs devices.

Conditioned reflex a complex adaptive reaction of the body that occurs due to the formation of a temporary neural connection (association) between a signal (conditioned) and reinforcing it with an unconditioned stimulus.

Conditioned reflexes are formed on the basis of innate unconditioned reflexes. Conditioned reflexes are individual, acquired reflex reactions that are produced on the basis of unconditioned reflexes. Their signs:

  1. Acquired throughout the life of the organism.
  2. They are not the same among representatives of the same species.
  3. They do not have ready-made reflex arcs.
  4. They are formed under certain conditions.
  5. In their implementation, the main role belongs to the cerebral cortex.
  6. Changeable, easily arise and disappear easily depending on the conditions in which the body is located.

Conditions for the formation of conditioned reflexes:

  1. The simultaneous action of two stimuli: indifferent for a given type of activity, which later becomes a conditioned signal, and an unconditioned stimulus, which causes a certain unconditioned reflex.
  2. The action of the conditioned stimulus always precedes the action of the unconditioned (by 1-5 s.).
  3. Reinforcement of the conditioned stimulus with the unconditioned must be repeated.
  4. The unconditioned stimulus must be biologically strong, and the conditioned stimulus must have moderate optimal strength.
  5. Conditioned reflexes are formed faster and easier in the absence of extraneous stimuli.

Conditioned reflexes can be produced not only on the basis of unconditioned ones, but also on the basis of previously acquired conditioned reflexes that have become quite strong. These are conditioned reflexes higher order. Conditioned reflexes are:

  • natural - reflex reactions that are produced to changes in the environment, and always accompany the emergence of the unconditional. For example, the smell and appearance of food are natural signals of the food itself;
  • artificial - conditioned reflexes produced in response to stimulation, which have no natural relation to the unconditional reflex reaction. For example, salivation for a call or for a while.

The conditioned reflex method is a method for studying GNI. I. P. Pavlov drew attention to the fact that the activity of the higher parts of the brain is not only associated with the direct influence of stimuli that have biological significance for the body, but also depends on the conditions that accompany these stimuli. For example, a dog begins to salivate not only when food enters its mouth, but also at the sight and smell of food, as soon as it sees the person who always brings it food. I.P. Pavlov explained this phenomenon by developing the method of conditioned reflexes. Using the method of conditioned reflexes, he conducted experiments on dogs with a fistula (stomy) of the excretory duct of the parotid salivary gland. The animal was offered two stimuli: food, a stimulus that has biological significance and causes salivation; the second is indifferent to the nutrition process (light, sound). These stimuli were combined in time so that the effect of light (sound) preceded the intake of food by several seconds. After a number of repetitions, saliva began to be released when the light bulb flashed and there was no food. Light (an indifferent stimulus) was called conditioned, since it is the condition under which food intake took place. A stimulus that has biological significance (food) is called unconditioned, and the physiological reaction of salivation, which occurs as a result of the action of a conditioned stimulus, is called a conditioned reflex.

To find out the mechanism of formation of conditioned reflexes, partial isolation of certain parts of the cerebral cortex and recording of the electrical activity of various brain structures during the action of unconditioned and conditioned stimuli are used.

I.P. Pavlov believed that with simultaneous action on two different analyzers in different sensitive areas of the cerebral hemispheres, excitation occurs, and over time, a connection is formed between them. For example, when a light bulb lights up and this stimulus is reinforced with food, excitation occurs in the cortical part of the visual analyzer, located in the occipital region of the cortex and excitation of the food center of the cerebral cortex - that is, in both cortical centers (visual and food), between which a nerve connection is formed. , which, with repeated combinations of these stimuli over time, becomes durable.

With conditioned reflexes, as with unconditioned reflexes, reverse afferentia occurs, that is, a signal that a conditioned reflex reaction has taken place. It allows the central nervous system to evaluate behavioral acts. Without such an assessment, subtle adaptation of behavior to constantly changing environmental conditions is impossible.

Studies of animals in which areas of the cortex were removed showed that conditioned reflexes could be developed in these animals. So, conditioned reflexes are formed as a result of the interaction of the cerebral cortex and subcortical centers. The structure of the reflex arc of a conditioned reflex is complex. Thus, in the formation of complex behavioral reactions, the cortex has a leading role, and in the formation of autonomic conditioned reflexes, the cortex and subcortical structures play the same role. It has been proven that the destruction of the reticular formation delays the formation of conditioned reflexes, and irritation with an electric current accelerates their formation. What are the signals of the conditioned reflex? Any changes in the environment or internal state of the body can become a conditioned stimulus if they:

  1. they themselves do not cause an unconditioned reflex; they are indifferent.
  2. their strength is sufficient to evoke an unconditioned orienting reflex.

For example, sounds, light, colors, smells, tastes, touch, pressure, heat, cold, body position in space - all these and others "indifferent" stimuli, when combined with an unconditioned stimulus and provided they are of sufficient strength, become signals that evoke one or another unconditioned reflex.

Biological significance of conditioned reflexes

The biological significance of conditioned reflexes lies in the fact that they are adaptive reactions of the body, which are formed by the living conditions of a person and make it possible to adapt in advance to new conditions. Conditioned reflexes have a warning signal value, since the body begins to react purposefully before a vital stimulus begins to act. Therefore, conditioned reflexes provide a living creature with the opportunity to assess danger or a red stimulus in advance, as well as the ability to carry out purposeful actions and consciously avoid mistakes.

10 questions in biology on the topic: unconditioned and conditioned reflexes.

  1. What are unconditioned reflexes? "Unconditioned reflexes" - These are specific, innate, relatively constant reactions of the body to the influence of the external and internal environment, carried out with the help of the nervous system.
  2. What are the main types of unconditioned reflexes? The main types of unconditioned reflexes include respiratory, food, grasping, protective, orientation and sexual.
  3. What are instincts? A complex system of innate (insanely reflexive) behavior programs associated with the preservation of the species is called instincts (from the Latin instinctus - urge, motive).
  4. What are conditioned reflexes? Conditioned reflexes, in contrast to unconditioned ones, are individual, arise during a person’s life, and are characteristic only of it; are temporary and may decline with changing environmental conditions.
  5. What conditions are needed for the formation of conditioned reflexes? Conditioned reflexes are formed on the basis of unconditioned ones.
  6. The mechanism of formation of conditioned reflexes? I.P. Pavlov found that the formation of conditioned reflexes is based on the establishment of temporary connections in the cerebral cortex between the nerve centers of the unconditioned reflex and the conditioned stimulus.
  7. What are the types of conditioned reflexes? natural - reflex reactions that are produced to changes in the environment, and always accompany the emergence of the unconditional. For example, the smell and appearance of food are natural signals of the food itself; artificial - conditioned reflexes produced in response to stimulation, which have no natural relation to the unconditional reflex reaction. For example, salivation for a call or for a while.
  8. Examples of unconditioned reflexes: blinking, breathing, reaction to sounds (indicative reflex), knee reflex.
  9. Examples of conditioned reflexes of recognizing food by smell, the processes of standing, running, walking, speaking, writing, and labor actions.
  10. Defensive reflexes are
    1. Unconditional.
    2. Conditional (conditional play a lesser role in defense)


Unconditioned reflex. Higher nervous activity. Unconditioned and conditioned reflexes

UNCONDITIONED REFLEX (specific, innate reflex) - a constant and innate reaction of the body to certain influences of the external world, carried out with the help of the nervous system and does not require special conditions for its occurrence. The term was introduced by I.P. Pavlov while studying the physiology of higher nervous activity. An unconditioned reflex occurs unconditionally if adequate stimulation is applied to a certain receptor surface. In contrast to this unconditionally occurring reflex, I.P. Pavlov discovered a category of reflexes, for the formation of which a number of conditions must be met - a conditioned reflex (see).

A physiological feature of the unconditioned reflex is its relative constancy. An unconditioned reflex always occurs with corresponding external or internal stimulation, manifesting itself on the basis of innate nerve connections. Since the constancy of the corresponding unconditioned reflex is the result of the phylogenetic development of a given animal species, this reflex received the additional name “species reflex”.

The biological and physiological role of the unconditioned reflex is that, thanks to this innate reaction, animals of a given species adapt (in the form of expedient acts of behavior) to the constant factors of existence.

The division of reflexes into two categories - unconditioned and conditioned - corresponds to two forms of nervous activity in animals and humans, which were clearly distinguished by I. P. Pavlov. The totality of the unconditioned reflex constitutes lower nervous activity, while the totality of acquired, or conditioned, reflexes constitutes higher nervous activity (see).

From this definition it follows that the unconditioned reflex, in its physiological meaning, along with the implementation of constant adaptive reactions of the animal in relation to the action of environmental factors, also determines those interactions of nervous processes that in total direct the internal life of the organism. This last property of the unconditioned reflex was given by I. P. Pavlov especially great value. Thanks to innate nerve connections that ensure the interaction of organs and processes within the body, animals and humans acquire an accurate and stable flow of basic vital functions. important functions. The principle on the basis of which these interactions and integration of activities within the body are organized is self-regulation of physiological functions (see).

Classification unconditioned reflexes can be built on the basis of the specific properties of the current stimulus and the biological meaning of the responses. It was on this principle that the classification was built in the laboratory of I. P. Pavlov. In accordance with this, there are several types of unconditioned reflex:

1. Food, the causative agent of which is the action of nutrients on the receptors of the tongue and on the basis of the study of which all the basic laws of higher nervous activity are formulated. Due to the spread of excitation from the receptors of the tongue towards the central nervous system, excitation of branched innate nervous structures occurs, which generally constitute the food center; As a result of such a fixed relationship between the central nervous system and the working peripheral apparatuses, responses of the whole organism are formed in the form of an unconditioned food reflex.

2. Defensive, or, as it is sometimes called, protective reflex. This unconditioned reflex has a number of forms depending on which organ or part of the body is in danger. For example, applying painful stimulation to a limb causes the limb to be withdrawn, which protects it from further destructive effects.

In a laboratory setting, electric current from appropriate devices (Dubois-Reymond induction coil, city current with a corresponding voltage drop, etc.) is usually used as a stimulus that evokes a defensive unconditioned reflex. If air movement directed at the cornea of ​​the eye is used as a stimulus, then the defensive reflex is manifested by closing the eyelids - the so-called blink reflex. If the irritants are potent gaseous substances that pass through the upper respiratory tract, then the protective reflex will be a delay in respiratory excursions chest. The most common type of protective reflex in the laboratory of I.P. Pavlov is the acid protective reflex. It is expressed by a strong rejection reaction (vomiting) in response to the infusion of a solution of hydrochloric acid into the animal’s oral cavity.

3. Sexual, which certainly occurs in the form of sexual behavior in response to an adequate sexual stimulus in the form of an individual of the opposite sex.

4. Indicative-exploratory, which is manifested by a quick movement of the head towards the one who acted in at the moment external stimulus. The biological meaning of this reflex consists in a detailed examination of the stimulus that acted and, in general, the external environment in which this stimulus arose. Thanks to the presence of innate pathways of this reflex in the central nervous system, the animal is able to respond expediently to sudden changes in the external world (see Orienting-exploratory reaction).

5. Reflexes from internal organs, reflexes during irritation of muscles and tendons (see Visceral reflexes, Tendon reflexes).

A common property of all unconditioned reflexes is that they can serve as the basis for the formation of acquired, or conditioned, reflexes. Some of the unconditioned reflexes, for example, defensive, lead to the formation of conditioned reactions very quickly, often after just one combination of any external stimulus with painful reinforcement. The ability of other unconditioned reflexes, for example, blinking or knee reflexes, to form temporary connections with an indifferent external stimulus is less pronounced.

It should also be taken into account that the rate of development of conditioned reflexes is directly dependent on the strength of the unconditioned stimulus.

The specificity of unconditioned reflexes lies in the exact correspondence of the body's response to the nature of the stimulus acting on the receptor apparatus. So, for example, when the taste buds of the tongue are irritated by a certain food, the reaction of the salivary glands in terms of the quality of the secretion is in strict accordance with the physical and chemical properties of the food taken. If the food is dry, then watery saliva is released, but if the food is sufficiently moistened, but consists of pieces (for example, bread), the unconditioned salivary reflex will manifest itself in accordance with this quality of food: saliva will contain large number mucous glucoprotein - mucin, which prevents injury to the food tract.

Fine receptor assessment is associated with a lack of a particular substance in the blood, for example, the so-called calcium starvation in children during the period of bone formation. Since calcium selectively passes through the capillaries of developing bones, eventually its amount becomes below a constant level. This factor is a selective irritant of some specific cells of the hypothalamus, which in turn keeps the receptors of the tongue in a state of increased excitability. This is how children develop a desire to eat plaster, whitewash and other minerals containing calcium.

Such an appropriate correspondence of the unconditioned reflex to the quality and strength of the stimulus that acts depends on the extremely differentiated effect of nutrients and their combinations on the receptors of the tongue. Receiving these combinations of afferent excitations from the periphery, the central apparatus of the unconditioned reflex sends peripheral devices(glands, muscles) efferent excitations leading to the formation of a certain composition of saliva or the occurrence of movements. In fact, the composition of saliva can be easily changed through a relative change in the production of its main ingredients: water, proteins, salts. It follows from this that the central salivary apparatus can vary the quantity and quality of excited elements depending on the quality of excitation coming from the periphery. The correspondence of an unconditioned response to the specificity of the applied stimulation can go very far. I. P. Pavlov developed the idea of ​​the so-called digestive warehouse of certain unconditioned reactions. For example, if you feed an animal a certain type of food for a long time, the digestive juices of its glands (stomach, pancreas, etc.) eventually acquire a certain composition in terms of the amount of water, inorganic salts, and especially the activity of enzymes. Such a “digestive warehouse” cannot but be recognized as an expedient adaptation of innate reflexes to the established constancy of food reinforcement.

At the same time, these examples indicate that the stability, or immutability, of the unconditioned reflex is only relative. There is reason to think that already in the first days after birth, the specific “mood” of the tongue receptors is prepared by the embryonic development of animals, which ensures the successful selection of nutrients and the planned course of unconditioned reactions. So, if the percentage of sodium chloride in the mother's milk that a newborn baby is fed on is increased, then the baby's sucking movements are immediately inhibited, and in some cases the baby actively throws out the formula that has already been taken. This example convinces us that the innate properties of food receptors, as well as the properties of intranervous relationships, most accurately reflect the needs of the newborn.

Methodology for using unconditioned reflexes

Since in the practice of work on higher nervous activity the unconditioned reflex is a reinforcing factor and the basis for the development of acquired, or conditioned, reflexes, the question of methodological techniques the use of the unconditioned reflex becomes especially important. In experiments on conditioned reflexes, the use of an unconditioned food reflex is based on feeding the animal with certain nutrients from an automatically fed feeder. With this method of using an unconditioned stimulus, the direct effect of food on the receptors of the animal’s tongue is inevitably preceded by a number of side irritations of the receptors related to various analyzers (see).

No matter how technically perfect the feeding of the feeder is, it certainly produces some kind of noise or knocking and, therefore, this sound stimulus is the inevitable precursor of the truest unconditioned stimulus, that is, the stimulus of the taste buds of the tongue. To eliminate these defects, a technique was developed for the direct introduction of nutrients into the oral cavity, while irrigation of the taste buds of the tongue, for example, with a sugar solution, is a direct unconditioned stimulus, not complicated by any side agent.

It should be noted, however, that under natural conditions animals and humans never receive food into the oral cavity without preliminary sensations (sight, smell of food, etc.). Therefore, the method of directly introducing food into the mouth has some abnormal conditions and the animal’s reaction to the unusual nature of such a procedure.

In addition to this use of an unconditioned stimulus, there are a number of techniques in which the animal itself receives food with the help of special movements. These include a wide variety of devices with the help of which an animal (rat, dog, monkey) receives food by pressing the corresponding lever or button - the so-called instrumental reflexes.

The methodological features of reinforcement with an unconditioned stimulus have an undoubted influence on the experimental results obtained, and, therefore, the assessment of the results should be made taking into account the type of unconditioned reflex. This especially applies to the comparative assessment of the food and defensive unconditioned reflex.

While reinforcement with a food unconditioned stimulus is a factor of positive biological significance for an animal (I.P. Pavlov), on the contrary, reinforcement with a painful stimulus is a stimulus for a biologically negative unconditioned reaction. It follows that “non-reinforcement” of a well-established conditioned reflex with an unconditioned stimulus in both cases will have the opposite biological sign. While non-reinforcement of a conditioned stimulus with food leads to a negative and often aggressive reaction in the experimental animal, on the contrary, non-reinforcement of the conditioned signal electric shock leads to a completely clear biological positive reaction. These features of the animal’s attitude to the non-reinforcement of a conditioned reflex by one or another unconditioned stimulus can be clearly identified by such a vegetative component as breathing.

Composition and localization of unconditioned reflexes

The development of experimental technology has made it possible to study the physiological composition and localization of the unconditioned food reflex in the central nervous system. For this purpose, the very effect of an unconditioned food stimulus on the receptors of the tongue was studied. Unconditioned stimulus regardless of its nutritional properties and consistency primarily irritates the tactile receptors of the tongue. This is the fastest type of excitation that is part of unconditioned stimulation. Tactile receptors produce the fastest and highest-amplitude type of nerve impulses, which first spread along the lingual nerve to the medulla oblongata, and only after a few fractions of a second (0.3 seconds) nerve impulses from temperature and chemical stimulation of the tongue receptors arrive there. This feature of the unconditioned stimulus, manifested in the sequential excitation of various receptors of the tongue, has enormous physiological significance: conditions are created in the central nervous system for signaling with each previous stream of impulses about subsequent stimuli. Thanks to such relationships and characteristics of tactile excitation, depending on the mechanical qualities of a given food, in response only to these excitations, salivation can occur before the chemical qualities of the food act.

Special experiments carried out on dogs and studies of the behavior of newborn children have shown that such relationships between individual parameters of the unconditioned stimulus are used in the adaptive behavior of the newborn.

For example, in the first days after birth, the decisive stimulus for a child’s food intake is its chemical qualities. However, after a few weeks, the leading role passes to the mechanical properties of food.

In the lives of adults, information about the tactile parameters of food is faster than information about chemical parameters in the brain. Thanks to this pattern, the sensation of “porridge”, “sugar”, etc. is born before the chemical signal arrives in the brain. According to the teachings of I.P. Pavlov on the cortical representation of the unconditioned reflex, each unconditioned irritation, along with the inclusion of the subcortical apparatuses, has its own representation in the cerebral cortex. Based on the above data, as well as oscillographic and electroencephalographic analysis of the spread of unconditioned excitation, it was established that it does not have a single point or focus in the cerebral cortex. Each of the fragments of unconditional excitation (tactile, temperature, chemical) is addressed to different points of the cerebral cortex, and only the almost simultaneous stimulation of these points of the cerebral cortex establishes a systemic connection between them. These new data correspond to I. P. Pavlov’s ideas about the structure of the nerve center, but require a change in existing ideas about the “cortical point” of the unconditioned stimulus.

Studies of cortical processes using electrical appliances showed that the unconditioned stimulus comes to the cerebral cortex in the form of a very generalized flow of ascending excitations, and, obviously, to each cell of the cortex. This means that not a single excitation of the sensory organs that preceded the unconditioned stimulus can “escape” its convergence with the unconditioned excitation. These properties of the unconditioned stimulus strengthen the idea of ​​“convergent closure” of the conditioned reflex.

Cortical representations of unconditioned reactions are cellular complexes that take an active part in the formation of a conditioned reflex, that is, in the closing functions of the cerebral cortex. By its nature, the cortical representation of the unconditioned reflex must be afferent in nature. As you know, I.P. Pavlov considered the cerebral cortex “an isolated afferent section of the central nervous system.”

Complex unconditioned reflexes. I.P. Pavlov identified a special category of unconditioned reflex, into which he included innate activities that are cyclical and behavioral in nature - emotions, instincts and other manifestations of complex acts of innate activity of animals and humans.

According to the initial opinion of I.P. Pavlov, complex unconditioned reflexes are a function of the “proximal subcortex”. Underneath this general expression refers to the thalamus, hypothalamus and other parts of the interstitial and midbrain. However, later, with the development of ideas about the cortical representations of the unconditioned reflex, this point of view was transferred to the concept of complex unconditioned reflexes. Thus, a complex unconditioned reflex, for example, an emotional discharge, has a specific subcortical part, but at the same time the very course of this complex unconditioned reflex at each individual stage has a representation in the cerebral cortex. This point of view of I.P. Pavlov was confirmed by research recent years using the neurography method. It has been shown that a number of cortical areas, for example, the orbital cortex, the limbic area, are directly related to the emotional manifestations of animals and humans.

According to I.P. Pavlov, complex unconditioned reflexes (emotions) represent a “blind force” or “the main source of strength” for cortical cells. The propositions expressed by I. P. Pavlov about complex unconditioned reflexes and their role in the formation of conditioned reflexes at that time were only at the stage of the most general development, and only in connection with the discovery of the physiological characteristics of the hypothalamus, the reticular formation of the brain stem, did a more in-depth study of this problems.

From the point of view of I.P. Pavlov, the instinctive activity of animals, including several various stages animal behavior is also a complex unconditioned reflex. The features of this type of unconditioned reflex are that the individual stages of performing any instinctive action are connected with each other according to the principle of a chain reflex; however, it was later shown that each such stage of behavior must necessarily have a reverse afferentation) from the results of the action itself, that is, carry out the process of comparing the actually obtained result with the previously predicted one. Only after this can the next stage of behavior be formed.

In the process of studying the unconditioned pain reflex, it was revealed that pain excitation undergoes significant transformations at the level of the brain stem and hypothalamus. From these structures, unconditioned excitation generally covers all areas of the cerebral cortex simultaneously. Thus, along with the mobilization in the cerebral cortex of systemic connections that are characteristic of a given unconditional excitation and form the basis of the cortical representation of the unconditioned reflex, unconditioned stimulation also produces a generalized effect on the entire cerebral cortex. In electroencephalographic analysis of cortical activity, this generalized effect of an unconditioned stimulus on the cerebral cortex manifests itself in the form of desynchronization of cortical wave electrical activity. The conduction of unconditioned painful excitation to the cerebral cortex can be blocked at the level of the brain stem using a special substance - aminazine. After the introduction of this substance into the blood, even strong damaging (nociceptive) unconditioned excitation (burn hot water) does not reach the cerebral cortex and does not change its electrical activity.

Development of unconditioned reflexes in the embryonic period

The innate nature of the unconditioned reflex is especially clearly revealed in studies of the embryonic development of animals and humans. On different dates embryogenesis, it is possible to trace each stage of the structural and functional formation of the unconditioned reflex. The vital functional systems of a newborn are completely consolidated at the time of birth. Individual parts of a sometimes complex unconditioned reflex, such as the sucking reflex, involve different parts of the body, often at a considerable distance from each other. Nevertheless, they are selectively united by various connections and gradually form a functional whole. The study of the maturation of the unconditioned reflex in embryogenesis makes it possible to understand the constant and relatively unchangeable adaptive effect of the unconditioned reflex upon application of the corresponding stimulus. This property of an unconditioned reflex is associated with the formation of interneuronal relationships based on morphogenetic and genetic patterns.

The maturation of the unconditioned reflex in the embryonic period is not the same for all animals. Since the maturation of the functional systems of the embryo has the most important biological meaning in preserving the life of a newborn of a given species of animal, then, depending on the characteristics of the conditions of existence of each species of animal, the nature of structural maturation and the final formation of the unconditioned reflex will exactly correspond to the characteristics of the given species.

For example, the structural design of spinal coordination reflexes turns out to be different in birds that, after hatching from an egg, immediately become completely independent (chicken), and in birds that, after hatching from an egg, are helpless for a long time and are in the care of their parents (rook). While a chick stands on its feet immediately after hatching and uses them completely freely every other day, in a rook, on the contrary, the forelimbs, that is, the wings, come into action first.

This selective growth of the nervous structures of the unconditioned reflex occurs even more clearly in the development of the human fetus. The very first and clearly visible motor reaction of the human fetus is the grasping reflex; it is detected already in the 4th month of intrauterine life and is caused by the application of any hard object to the palm of the fetus. Morphological analysis of all links of this reflex convinces us that, before it is revealed, a number of nerve structures differentiate into mature neurons and unite with each other. Myelination of the nerve trunks related to the finger flexors begins and ends earlier than this process unfolds in the nerve trunks of other muscles.

Phylogenetic development of unconditioned reflexes

According to the well-known position of I.P. Pavlov, unconditioned reflexes are a consequence of the consolidation by natural selection and heredity of those reactions acquired over thousands of years that correspond to repeated environmental factors and are useful for a given species.

There is reason to assert that the most rapid and successful adaptations of the organism may depend on favorable mutations, which are subsequently selected by natural selection and are already inherited.

Bibliography: Anokhin P.K. Biology and neurophysiology of the conditioned reflex, M., 1968, bibliogr.; Afferent link of interoceptive reflexes, ed. I. A. Bulygina, M., 1964; Vedyaev F. P. Subcortical mechanisms of complex motor reflexes, JI., 1965, bibliogr.; Vinogradova O. S. Orienting reflex and its neurophysiological mechanisms, M., 1961, bibliogr.; Groysman S. D. and Dekush P. G. An attempt at a quantitative study of intestinal reflexes, Pat. physiol. and Experiment, ter., v. 3, p. 51, 1974, bibliogr.; Orbeli JI. A. Questions of higher nervous activity, p. 146, M.-JI., 1949; Pavlov I.P. Complete works, vol. 1-6, M., 1951 - 1952; Petukhov B. N. Closure after loss of basic unconditioned reflexes, Proceedings Center, Institute of Improvements. doctors, vol. 81, p. 54, M., 1965, bibliogr.; S a l h e nko I. N. Hidden periods of myotatic reflexes that ensure motor interactions of people, Physiol. human, vol. 1, Jvft 2, p. 317, 197 5, bibliography; Sechenov I. M. Reflexes of the brain, M., 1961; Slonim A.D. Fundamentals of general economic physiology of mammals, p. 72, M,-JI., 1961, bibliogr.; Human Physiology, ed. E. B. Babsky, p. 592, M., 1972; Frankstein S.I. Respiratory reflexes and mechanisms of shortness of breath, M., 1974, bibliogr.; Sh u s t i n N. A. Analysis of unconditioned reflexes in the light of the doctrine of the dominant, Physiol, journal. USSR, vol. 61, JSft 6, p. 855, 1975, bibliogr.; Human reflexes, pathophysiology of motor systems, ed. by J. E. Desment, Basel a. o., 1973; Mechanisms of orienting reactions in man, ed. by I. Ruttkay-Nedecky a. o., Bratislava, 1967.

Conditioned reflexes are complex adaptive reactions of the body, carried out by the higher parts of the central nervous system through the formation of a temporary connection between a signal stimulus and an unconditioned reflex act that reinforces this stimulus. Based on an analysis of the patterns of formation of conditioned reflexes, the school created the doctrine of higher nervous activity (see). Unlike unconditioned reflexes (see), which ensure the body’s adaptation to constant environmental influences, conditioned reflexes enable the body to adapt to changing environmental conditions. Conditioned reflexes are formed on the basis of unconditioned reflexes, which requires the coincidence in time of some stimulus from the external environment (conditioned stimulus) with the implementation of one or another unconditioned reflex. The conditioned stimulus becomes a signal of a dangerous or favorable situation, allowing the body to respond with an adaptive reaction.

Conditioned reflexes are not permanent and are acquired in the process individual development body. Conditioned reflexes are divided into natural and artificial. The first ones arise in response to natural stimuli in natural conditions of existence: a puppy, having received meat for the first time, sniffs it for a long time and timidly eats it, and this act of eating is accompanied by. In the future, only the sight and smell of meat causes the puppy to lick and eliminate. Artificial conditioned reflexes are developed in an experimental setting, when the conditioned stimulus for an animal is an influence that is not related to unconditioned reactions in the animals’ natural habitat (for example, flickering light, the sound of a metronome, sound clicks).

Conditioned reflexes are divided into food, defensive, sexual, orienting, depending on the unconditional reaction that reinforces the conditioned stimulus. Conditioned reflexes can be named depending on the registered response of the body: motor, secretory, vegetative, excretory, and can also be designated by the type of conditioned stimulus - light, sound, etc.

To develop conditioned reflexes in an experiment, a number of conditions are necessary: ​​1) the conditioned stimulus must always precede the unconditioned stimulus in time; 2) the conditioned stimulus should not be strong so as not to cause the body’s own reaction; 3) a conditioned stimulus is taken that is usually found in the environmental conditions of the given animal or person; 4) the animal or person must be healthy, cheerful and have sufficient motivation (see).

There are also conditioned reflexes of various orders. When a conditioned stimulus is reinforced by an unconditioned stimulus, a first-order conditioned reflex is developed. If some stimulus is reinforced by a conditioned stimulus to which a conditioned reflex has already been developed, then a second-order conditioned reflex is developed to the first stimulus. Conditioned reflexes of higher orders are developed with difficulty, which depends on the level of organization of the living organism.

A dog can develop conditioned reflexes of up to 5-6 orders, in a monkey - up to 10-12 orders, in humans - up to 50-100 orders.

The work of I.P. Pavlov and his students established that in the mechanism of the emergence of conditioned reflexes, the leading role belongs to the formation of a functional connection between the foci of excitation from conditioned and unconditioned stimuli. An important role was assigned to the cerebral cortex, where conditioned and unconditioned stimuli, creating foci of excitation, began to interact with each other, creating temporary connections. Subsequently, using electrophysiological research methods, it was established that the interaction between conditioned and unconditioned excitations can first occur at the level of subcortical structures of the brain, and at the level of the cerebral cortex, the formation of integral conditioned reflex activity takes place.

However, the cerebral cortex always controls the activity of subcortical formations.

By studying the activity of single neurons of the central nervous system using the microelectrode method, it was established that both conditioned and unconditioned excitations come to one neuron (sensory-biological convergence). It is especially clearly expressed in the neurons of the cerebral cortex. These data forced us to abandon the idea of ​​the presence of foci of conditioned and unconditioned excitation in the cerebral cortex and create the theory of convergent closure of the conditioned reflex. According to this theory, a temporary connection between conditioned and unconditioned excitation arises in the form of a chain of biochemical reactions in protoplasm nerve cell cerebral cortex.

Modern ideas about conditioned reflexes have expanded and deepened significantly thanks to the study of the higher nervous activity of animals in conditions of their free natural behavior. It has been established that the environment, along with the time factor, plays an important role in the behavior of the animal. Any stimulus from the external environment can become conditioned, allowing the body to adapt to environmental conditions. As a result of the formation of conditioned reflexes, the body reacts some time before the impact of unconditioned stimulation. Consequently, conditioned reflexes contribute to the successful finding of food by animals, help to avoid danger in advance and most perfectly navigate the changing conditions of existence.

Continuation. See No. 34, 35, 36/2004

Congenital and acquired forms of behavior

Lessons on the topic: “Physiology of higher nervous activity”

Table. Comparison of unconditioned and conditioned reflexes

Signs of comparison

Unconditioned reflexes

Conditioned reflexes

Inheritance

Congenital, passed on from parents to offspring

Acquired by the body during life, they are not inherited

Species specificity

Individual

Stimulus

Carried out in response to an unconditioned stimulus

Carried out in response to any irritation perceived by the body; are formed on the basis of unconditioned reflexes

Meaning in life

Life without them is usually impossible

Promote the survival of the organism in constantly changing environmental conditions

Duration of existence of a reflex arc

Have ready and permanent reflex arcs

They do not have ready-made and permanent reflex arcs; their arcs are temporary and form under certain conditions

Reflex centers

They are carried out at the level of the spinal cord, brain stem and subcortical nuclei, i.e. reflex arcs pass through the lower levels of the central nervous system

They are carried out due to the activity of the cerebral cortex, i.e. reflex arcs pass through the cerebral cortex

Lesson 5.
Generalization of knowledge on the topic “Acquired forms of behavior. Conditioned reflex"

Equipment: tables, diagrams and drawings illustrating acquired forms of behavior, mechanisms for the development of conditioned reflexes.

PROGRESS OF THE LESSON

I. Test of knowledge

Working with cards

1. The advantage of behavior formed as a result of learning is that it:

a) is carried out quickly;
b) is carried out the same way every time;
c) provides answers in changing environmental conditions;
d) done correctly the first time;
e) does not occupy a place in the genetic program of the organism.

2. For experiments on studying conditioned reflexes, two dogs were taken. One of them was given a large amount of water to drink. Then the research began. At first, conditioned reflexes were carried out normally in both dogs. But after some time, the conditioned reflexes disappeared in the dog that drank water. There were no random external influences. What is the reason for the inhibition of conditioned reflexes?

3. As is known, a conditioned reflex can be developed to the action of almost any indifferent stimulus. One dog in the laboratory of I.P. Pavlov never managed to develop a conditioned reflex to the gurgling of water. Try to explain the lack of results in this case.

4. It is known that the strength (biological significance) of the conditioned stimulus should not exceed the strength of the unconditioned stimulus. Otherwise, the conditioned reflex cannot be developed. Therefore, it is very difficult to develop, for example, a conditioned food reflex to painful stimulation (electric current). However, in the laboratory of I.P. In Pavlov’s famous experiments, Erofeeva managed to develop such a conditioned reflex. When exposed to a current (conditioned stimulus), the dog salivated, it licked its lips and wagged its tail. How did you achieve this?

5. During one of the concerts, a listener suddenly began to experience pain in the heart area. Moreover, the onset of pain coincided with the performance of one of Chopin’s nocturnes. Since then, every time the man heard this music, his heart ached. Explain this pattern.

Oral knowledge test on questions

1. Learning and its methods (habituation, trial and error).
2. Imprinting and its characteristics.
3. Methods for developing conditioned reflexes.
4. Mechanisms for the development of conditioned reflexes
5. General properties and classification of conditioned reflexes.
6. Rational activity of animals.
7. Dynamic stereotype and its meaning.

Checking the completion of the table “Comparison of unconditioned and conditioned reflexes”

The children had to fill out the table as homework after the previous lesson.

Biological dictation

The teacher reads out the characteristics of reflexes under numbers, and students, working on the options, write down the numbers of the correct answers: option I – unconditioned reflexes, option II – conditioned reflexes.

1. Passed on by inheritance.
2. Not inherited.
3. Reflex centers are located in the subcortical nuclei, brain stem and spinal cord.
4. Reflex centers are located in the cerebral cortex.
5. There is no species specificity; each individual of the species develops its own reflexes.
6. Species specificity - these reflexes are characteristic of all individuals of a certain species.
7. Stable throughout life.
8. Change (new reflexes arise, and old ones fade away).
9. The reasons for the formation of reflexes are events that are vital for the whole species.
10. The causes of reflexes are signals that arise from personal past experience and warn of an important event.

Answers: Option I – 1, 3, 6, 7, 9; Option II – 2, 4, 5, 8, 10.

Laboratory work No. 2.
“Development of conditioned reflexes in humans on the basis of unconditioned reflexes”

Equipment: rubber bulb for pumping air, metronome.

PROGRESS OF WORK

1. Turn on the metronome at a rhythm of 120 beats per minute and on its second or third beat, press the bulb, directing a stream of air into the subject’s eye.

2. Repeat the steps described in step 1 until the blinking steadily (at least 2-3 times in a row) precedes the pressing of the bulb.

3. After the blink reflex has been developed, turn on the metronome without directing the air stream to the eye. What do you observe? Draw a conclusion.

What reflex was developed in the subject during the actions you performed? What performs the role of unconditioned and conditioned stimuli in the developed reflex? What is the difference between the arcs of the unconditioned blink and conditioned blink reflexes?

Homework

Repeat the material about the mechanisms of development of conditioned reflexes in animals and humans.

Lesson 6–7.
Congenital and acquired inhibition, their types and characteristics

Equipment: tables, diagrams and drawings illustrating the mechanisms of development of conditioned reflexes, various types of congenital and acquired inhibition.

PROGRESS OF THE LESSON

I. Test of knowledge

Working with cards

1. Thanks to what innate nervous mechanisms can an animal distinguish good-quality food from spoiled food? What role do neurons and their synapses play in these processes?

2. What facts can be used to prove that instinct is a chain of interconnected unconditioned reflexes? How do instincts interact with acquired conditioned reflexes?

3. Infant when he sees a bottle of kefir, he smacks his lips; a person salivates when he sees a lemon being cut; Wanting to know what time it is, a man looks at his hand, where he usually wears his watch, although he forgot it at home. Explain the described phenomena.

Knowledge test

Choose the correct answers to the given statements.

1. This is an unconditioned stimulus.
2. It is an indifferent stimulus.
3. This is an unconditioned reflex.
4. This is a conditioned reflex.
5. This is a combination of an indifferent stimulus with an unconditioned one.
6. Without these stimuli, the conditioned salivary reflex is not formed.
7. Stimulus that excites the visual cortex.
8. An irritant that excites the gustatory cortex.
9. Under this condition, a temporary connection is formed between the visual and gustatory zones of the cortex.

Answer options

A. Turning on the light bulb before experiments without feeding.
B. Food in the mouth.
B. Turning on the light during feeding.
D. Salivation of food in the mouth.
D. Secretion of saliva to the light of a light bulb.

Answers: 1 – B, 2 – A, 3 – D, 4 – D, 5 – B, 6 – C, 7 – A, 8 – B, 9 – C.

II. Learning new material

1. Excitation and inhibition are the main processes of nervous activity

As you already know, the regulatory function of the central nervous system is carried out through two processes - excitation and inhibition.

Conversation with students on issues

    What is excitement?

    What is braking?

    Why is the process of excitation called the active state of nervous tissue?

    What does excitation of motor centers lead to?

    Thanks to what process can we mentally imagine them without performing any actions?

    What processes enable complex coordinated actions such as walking?

Thus, excitation– this is an active state of nervous tissue in response to the action of various stimuli of sufficient strength. When excited, neurons generate electrical impulses. Braking- This is an active nervous process leading to inhibition of excitation.

2. General characteristics of cortical inhibition

Excitation and inhibition of I.P. Pavlov called them the true creators of nervous activity.

Excitation is involved in the formation of conditioned reflexes and their implementation. The role of inhibition is more complex and varied. It is the process of inhibition that makes conditioned reflexes a mechanism of subtle, precise and perfect adaptation to the environment.

According to I.P. Pavlov, the cortex is characterized by two forms of inhibition: unconditional and conditioned. Unconditional inhibition does not require development; it is inherent in the body from birth (reflexive holding of breath when there is a strong smell of ammonia, inhibition in the triceps brachii muscle during the action of the biceps brachii, etc.). Conditioned inhibition is developed in the process of individual experience.

The following types of braking are distinguished. Unconditional braking: beyond (protective); external; innate reflexes. Conditional braking: extinct; differentiation; delayed.

3. Types of unconditional (congenital) inhibition and their characteristics

In the process of life, the body is constantly exposed to one or another irritation from the outside or from the inside. Each of these irritations is capable of causing a corresponding reflex. If all these reflexes could be realized, then the body’s activity would be chaotic. However, this does not happen. On the contrary, reflex activity is characterized by consistency and orderliness: with the help of unconditional inhibition, the most important reflex for the body at a given moment delays all other, secondary reflexes for the duration of its implementation.

Depending on the reasons underlying the inhibition processes, the following types of unconditional inhibition are distinguished.

Transcendental, or protective, braking occurs in response to very strong stimuli that require the body to act beyond its capabilities. The strength of irritation is determined by the frequency of nerve impulses. The more excited the neuron, the higher the frequency of the flow nerve impulses it generates. But if this flow exceeds known limits, processes arise that prevent the passage of excitation along the chain of neurons. The flow of nerve impulses following the reflex arc is interrupted, and inhibition occurs, which protects the executive organs from exhaustion.

Cause of external braking is outside the structures of the inhibitory reflex, it comes from another reflex. This type of inhibition occurs whenever a new activity is started. The new excitation, being stronger, causes inhibition of the old one. As a result, the previous activity is automatically terminated. For example, a dog has developed a strong conditioned reflex to light, and the lecturer wants to demonstrate it to the audience. The experiment fails - there is no reflex. An unfamiliar environment, the noise of a crowded audience are new signals that completely turn off conditioned reflex activity, and new excitation arises in the cortex. If the dog is brought into the audience several times, then new signals, which turn out to be biologically indifferent, fade away, and conditioned reflexes are carried out unhindered.

To be continued

CATEGORIES

POPULAR ARTICLES

2024 “mobi-up.ru” - Garden plants. Interesting things about flowers. Perennial flowers and shrubs